Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 45
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Front Psychol ; 4: 650, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24098287

RESUMO

People often feel like their minds and their bodies are in different places. Far from an exotic experience, this phenomenon seems to be a ubiquitous facet of human life (e.g., Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). Many times, people's minds seem to go "somewhere else"-attention becomes disconnected from perception, and people's minds wander to times and places removed from the current environment (e.g., Schooler et al., 2004). At other times, however, people's minds may seem to go nowhere at all-they simply disappear. This mental state-mind-blanking-may represent an extreme decoupling of perception and attention, one in which attention fails to bring any stimuli into conscious awareness. In the present research, we outline the properties of mind-blanking, differentiating this mental state from other mental states in terms of phenomenological experience, behavioral outcomes, and underlying cognitive processes. Seven experiments suggest that when the mind seems to disappear, there are times when we have simply failed to monitor its whereabouts-and there are times when it is actually gone.

2.
PLoS One ; 8(8): e72589, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24015263

RESUMO

It is important for robot designers to know how to make robots that interact effectively with humans. One key dimension is robot appearance and in particular how humanlike the robot should be. Uncanny Valley theory suggests that robots look uncanny when their appearance approaches, but is not absolutely, human. An underlying mechanism may be that appearance affects users' perceptions of the robot's personality and mind. This study aimed to investigate how robot facial appearance affected perceptions of the robot's mind, personality and eeriness. A repeated measures experiment was conducted. 30 participants (14 females and 16 males, mean age 22.5 years) interacted with a Peoplebot healthcare robot under three conditions in a randomized order: the robot had either a humanlike face, silver face, or no-face on its display screen. Each time, the robot assisted the participant to take his/her blood pressure. Participants rated the robot's mind, personality, and eeriness in each condition. The robot with the humanlike face display was most preferred, rated as having most mind, being most humanlike, alive, sociable and amiable. The robot with the silver face display was least preferred, rated most eerie, moderate in mind, humanlikeness and amiability. The robot with the no-face display was rated least sociable and amiable. There was no difference in blood pressure readings between the robots with different face displays. Higher ratings of eeriness were related to impressions of the robot with the humanlike face display being less amiable, less sociable and less trustworthy. These results suggest that the more humanlike a healthcare robot's face display is, the more people attribute mind and positive personality characteristics to it. Eeriness was related to negative impressions of the robot's personality. Designers should be aware that the face on a robot's display screen can affect both the perceived mind and personality of the robot.


Assuntos
Pressão Sanguínea , Face , Percepção , Personalidade , Robótica , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
3.
Psychol Sci ; 24(8): 1437-45, 2013 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23749051

RESUMO

People often think that something must have a mind to be part of a moral interaction. However, the present research suggests that minds do not create morality but that morality creates minds. In four experiments, we found that observing intentional harm to an unconscious entity--a vegetative patient, a robot, or a corpse--leads to augmented attribution of mind to that entity. A fifth experiment reconciled these results with extant research on dehumanization by showing that observing the victimization of conscious entities leads to reduced attribution of mind to those entities. Taken together, these experiments suggest that the effects of victimization vary according to victims' preexisting mental status and that people often make an intuitive cognitive error when unconscious entities are placed in harm's way. People assume that if apparent moral harm occurs, then there must be someone there to experience that harm-a harm-made mind. These findings have implications for political policies concerning right-to-life issues.


Assuntos
Estado de Consciência , Vítimas de Crime/psicologia , Percepção Social , Teoria da Mente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Princípios Morais
4.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 8(5): 549-53, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26173211

RESUMO

There are many guides on proper psychology, but far fewer on interesting psychology. This article presents six guidelines for interesting research. The first three-Phenomena First, Be Surprising, and Grandmothers, Not Scientists-suggest how to choose your research question; the last three-Be The Participant, Simple Statistics, and Powerful Beginnings-suggest how to answer your research question and offer perspectives on experimental design, statistical analysis, and effective communication. These guidelines serve as reminders that replicability is necessary but not sufficient for compelling psychological science. Interesting research considers subjective experience; it listens to the music of the human condition.

6.
Cognition ; 125(1): 125-30, 2012 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22784682

RESUMO

The uncanny valley-the unnerving nature of humanlike robots-is an intriguing idea, but both its existence and its underlying cause are debated. We propose that humanlike robots are not only unnerving, but are so because their appearance prompts attributions of mind. In particular, we suggest that machines become unnerving when people ascribe to them experience (the capacity to feel and sense), rather than agency (the capacity to act and do). Experiment 1 examined whether a machine's humanlike appearance prompts both ascriptions of experience and feelings of unease. Experiment 2 tested whether a machine capable of experience remains unnerving, even without a humanlike appearance. Experiment 3 investigated whether the perceived lack of experience can also help explain the creepiness of unfeeling humans and philosophical zombies. These experiments demonstrate that feelings of uncanniness are tied to perceptions of experience, and also suggest that experience-but not agency-is seen as fundamental to humans, and fundamentally lacking in machines.


Assuntos
Emoções , Deficiência Intelectual/psicologia , Robótica , Teoria da Mente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Conscious Cogn ; 21(1): 501-6, 2012 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22115726

RESUMO

The conscious awareness of voluntary action is associated with systematic changes in time perception: The interval between actions and outcomes is experienced as compressed in time. Although this temporal binding is thought to result from voluntary movement and provides a window to the sense of agency, recent studies challenge this idea by demonstrating binding in involuntary movement. We offer a potential account for these findings by proposing that binding between involuntary actions and effects can occur when self-causation is implied. Participants made temporal judgements concerning a key press and a tone, while they learned to consider themselves as the cause of the effect or not. Results showed that implied self-causation (vs. no implied self-causation) increased temporal binding. Since intrinsic motor cues of movement were absent, these results suggest that sensory evidence about the key press caused binding in retrospect and in line with the participant's sense of being an agent.


Assuntos
Associação , Movimento , Autoimagem , Percepção do Tempo , Volição , Adulto , Antecipação Psicológica , Conscientização , Causalidade , Retroalimentação Sensorial , Humanos
8.
Am Psychol ; 66(8): 671-80, 2011 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22082381

RESUMO

A person who is asked to think aloud while trying not to think about a white bear will typically mention the bear once a minute. So how can people suppress unwanted thoughts? This article examines a series of indirect thought suppression techniques and therapies that have been explored for their efficacy as remedies for unwanted thoughts of all kinds and that offer some potential as means for effective suppression. The strategies that have some promise include focused distraction, stress and load avoidance, thought postponement, exposure and paradoxical approaches, acceptance and commitment, meditation, mindfulness, focused breathing, attention training, self-affirmation, hypnosis, and disclosure and writing. Many of these strategies entail thinking about and accepting unwanted thoughts rather than suppressing them--and so, setting free the bears. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Terapia Comportamental/métodos , Meditação/métodos , Repressão Psicológica , Pensamento , Humanos
9.
Cognition ; 121(2): 275-80, 2011 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21798528

RESUMO

Patients in persistent vegetative state (PVS) may be biologically alive, but these experiments indicate that people see PVS as a state curiously more dead than dead. Experiment 1 found that PVS patients were perceived to have less mental capacity than the dead. Experiment 2 explained this effect as an outgrowth of afterlife beliefs, and the tendency to focus on the bodies of PVS patients at the expense of their minds. Experiment 3 found that PVS is also perceived as "worse" than death: people deem early death better than being in PVS. These studies suggest that people perceive the minds of PVS patients as less valuable than those of the dead - ironically, this effect is especially robust for those high in religiosity.


Assuntos
Morte , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/psicologia , Adulto , Atitude , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Processos Mentais , Percepção/fisiologia , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/fisiopatologia , Religião , Adulto Jovem
10.
Science ; 333(6043): 776-8, 2011 Aug 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21764755

RESUMO

The advent of the Internet, with sophisticated algorithmic search engines, has made accessing information as easy as lifting a finger. No longer do we have to make costly efforts to find the things we want. We can "Google" the old classmate, find articles online, or look up the actor who was on the tip of our tongue. The results of four studies suggest that when faced with difficult questions, people are primed to think about computers and that when people expect to have future access to information, they have lower rates of recall of the information itself and enhanced recall instead for where to access it. The Internet has become a primary form of external or transactive memory, where information is stored collectively outside ourselves.


Assuntos
Cognição , Armazenamento e Recuperação da Informação , Internet , Memória , Rememoração Mental , Ferramenta de Busca , Computadores , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Teste de Stroop
11.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 101(2): 233-44, 2011 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21463077

RESUMO

Naïve theories of behavior hold that actions are caused by an agent's intentions, and the subsequent success of an action is measured by the satisfaction of those intentions. However, when an action is not as successful as intended, the expected causal link between intention and action may distort perception of the action itself. Four studies found evidence of an intention bias in perceptions of action. Actors perceived actions to be more successful when given a prior choice (e.g., choose between 2 words to type) and also when they felt greater motivation for the action (e.g., hitting pictures of disliked people). When the intent was to fail (e.g., singing poorly), choice led to worse estimates of performance. A final experiment suggested that intention bias works independent from self-enhancement motives. In observing another actor hit pictures of Hillary Clinton and Barack Obama, shots were distorted to match the actor's intentions, even when it opposed personal wishes. Together these studies indicate that judgments of action may be automatically distorted and that these inferences arise from the expected consistency between intention and action in agency.


Assuntos
Logro , Intenção , Variações Dependentes do Observador , Autoimagem , Adulto , Comportamento de Escolha , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Motivação , Percepção , Testes Psicológicos , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 20(3): 965-71, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21367624

RESUMO

Belief in free will is widespread. The present research considered one reason why people may believe that actions are freely chosen rather than determined: they attribute randomness in behavior to free will. Experiment 1 found that participants who were prompted to perform a random sequence of actions experienced their behavior as more freely chosen than those who were prompted to perform a deterministic sequence. Likewise, Experiment 2 found that, all else equal, the behavior of animated agents was perceived to be more freely chosen if it consisted of a random sequence of actions than if it consisted of a deterministic sequence; this was true even when the degree of randomness in agents' behavior was largely a product of their environments. Together, these findings suggest that randomness in behavior--one's own or another's--can be mistaken for free will.


Assuntos
Autonomia Pessoal , Distribuição Aleatória , Comportamento de Escolha , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção , Percepção Social
13.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(2): 477-9, 2011 Jan 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21187372

RESUMO

It has long been known that psychopathology can influence social perception, but a 2D framework of mind perception provides the opportunity for an integrative understanding of some disorders. We examined the covariation of mind perception with three subclinical syndromes--autism-spectrum disorder, schizotypy, and psychopathy--and found that each presents a unique mind-perception profile. Autism-spectrum disorder involves reduced perception of agency in adult humans. Schizotypy involves increased perception of both agency and experience in entities generally thought to lack minds. Psychopathy involves reduced perception of experience in adult humans, children, and animals. Disorders are differentially linked with the over- or underperception of agency and experience in a way that helps explain their real-world consequences.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Percepção , Psicologia/métodos , Psicofisiologia , Adulto , Criança , Transtornos Globais do Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos , Transtorno da Personalidade Esquizotípica/fisiopatologia
14.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 14(8): 383-8, 2010 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20579932

RESUMO

Perceiving others' minds is a crucial component of social life. People do not, however, always ascribe minds to other people, and sometimes ascribe minds to non-people (e.g. God, gadgets). This article reviews when mind perception occurs, when it does not, and why mind perception is important. Causes of mind perception stem both from the perceiver and perceived, and include the need for social connection (perceiver) and a similarity to oneself (perceived). Mind perception also has profound consequences for both the perceiver and perceived. Ascribing mind confers an entity moral rights and also makes its actions meaningful. Understanding the causes and consequences of mind perception can explain when this most social of cognitive skills will be used, and why it matters.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Teoria da Mente , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Percepção/fisiologia
15.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 5(4): 392-403, 2010 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20150343

RESUMO

Mentalization is the process by which an observer views a target as possessing higher cognitive faculties such as goals, intentions and desires. Mentalization can be assessed using action identification paradigms, in which observers choose mentalistic (goals-focused) or mechanistic (action-focused) descriptions of targets' actions. Neural structures that play key roles in inferring goals and intentions from others' observed or imagined actions include temporo-parietal junction, ventral premotor cortex and extrastriate body area. We hypothesized that these regions play a role in action identification as well. Data collected using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) confirmed our predictions that activity in ventral premotor cortex and middle temporal gyrus near the extrastriate body area varies both as a function of the valence of the target and the extent to which actions are identified as goal-directed. In addition, the inferior parietal lobule is preferentially engaged when participants identify the actions of mentalized targets. Functional connectivity analyses suggest support from other regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala, during mentalization. We found correlations between action identification and Autism Quotient scores, suggesting that understanding the neural correlates of action identification may enhance our understanding of the underpinnings of essential social cognitive processes.


Assuntos
Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Adulto , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Feminino , Objetivos , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Testes Psicológicos , Percepção Social
16.
Conscious Cogn ; 19(1): 481-9, 2010 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19896868

RESUMO

It has been proposed that inferring personal authorship for an event gives rise to intentional binding, a perceptual illusion in which one's action and inferred effect seem closer in time than they otherwise would (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). Using a novel, naturalistic paradigm, we conducted two experiments to test this hypothesis and examine the relationship between binding and self-reported authorship. In both experiments, an important authorship indicator - consistency between one's action and a subsequent event - was manipulated, and its effects on binding and self-reported authorship were measured. Results showed that action-event consistency enhanced both binding and self-reported authorship, supporting the hypothesis that binding arises from an inference of authorship. At the same time, evidence for a dissociation emerged, with consistency having a more robust effect on self-reports than on binding. Taken together, these results suggest that binding and self-reports reveal different aspects of the sense of authorship.


Assuntos
Controle Interno-Externo , Julgamento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Autoimagem , Percepção do Tempo , Conscientização , Percepção de Forma , Humanos , Intenção , Modelos Psicológicos , Orientação , Autonomia Pessoal , Tempo de Reação
17.
Pers Soc Psychol Rev ; 14(1): 7-16, 2010 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19926831

RESUMO

Believing in God requires not only a leap of faith but also an extension of people's normal capacity to perceive the minds of others. Usually, people perceive minds of all kinds by trying to understand their conscious experience (what it is like to be them) and their agency (what they can do). Although humans are perceived to have both agency and experience, humans appear to see God as possessing agency, but not experience. God's unique mind is due, the authors suggest, to the uniquely moral role He occupies. In this article, the authors propose that God is seen as the ultimate moral agent, the entity people blame and praise when they receive anomalous harm and help. Support for this proposition comes from research on mind perception, morality, and moral typecasting. Interestingly, although people perceive God as the author of salvation, suffering seems to evoke even more attributions to the divine.


Assuntos
Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Princípios Morais , Dor/psicologia , Religião e Psicologia , Teoria da Mente , Cultura , Humanos , Controle Interno-Externo , Motivação , Teoria da Construção Pessoal , Justiça Social
18.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 38(1): 1-14, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19852877

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The belief that we can control our thoughts is not inevitably adaptive, particularly when it fuels mental control activities that have ironic unintended consequences. The conviction that the mind can and should be controlled can prompt people to suppress unwanted thoughts, and so can set the stage for the intrusive return of those very thoughts. An important question is whether or not these beliefs about the control of thoughts can be reduced experimentally. One possibility is that behavioral experiments aimed at revealing the ironic return of suppressed thoughts might create a lesson that could reduce unrealistic beliefs about the control of thoughts. AIMS: The present research assessed the influence of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts in a non-clinical sample, and among individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). METHOD: In Study 1, we assessed the effect of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts among low and high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population (N = 62). In Study 2, we conducted a similar study with individuals with OCD (N = 29). RESULTS: Results suggest that high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population are able to learn the futility of suppression through the thought suppression demonstration and to alter their faulty beliefs about the control of thoughts; however, for individuals with OCD, the demonstration may be insufficient for altering underlying beliefs. CONCLUSIONS: For individuals with OCD, the connection between suppressing a neutral thought in the suppression demonstration and suppressing a personally relevant obsession may need to be stated explicitly in order to affect their obsessive beliefs.


Assuntos
Inibição Psicológica , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/psicologia , Repressão Psicológica , Pensamento , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Conscientização , Cultura , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicometria , Adulto Jovem
19.
Science ; 325(5936): 48-50, 2009 Jul 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19574380

RESUMO

In slapstick comedy, the worst thing that could happen usually does: The person with a sore toe manages to stub it, sometimes twice. Such errors also arise in daily life, and research traces the tendency to do precisely the worst thing to ironic processes of mental control. These monitoring processes keep us watchful for errors of thought, speech, and action and enable us to avoid the worst thing in most situations, but they also increase the likelihood of such errors when we attempt to exert control under mental load (stress, time pressure, or distraction). Ironic errors in attention and memory occur with identifiable brain activity and prompt recurrent unwanted thoughts; attraction to forbidden desires; expression of objectionable social prejudices; production of movement errors; and rebounds of negative experiences such as anxiety, pain, and depression. Such ironies can be overcome when effective control strategies are deployed and mental load is minimized.


Assuntos
Emoções , Intenção , Processos Mentais , Pensamento , Ansiedade , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Medo , Humanos , Controle Interno-Externo , Memória , Movimento , Preconceito
20.
Conscious Cogn ; 18(4): 1056-64, 2009 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19515577

RESUMO

We investigate the processes underlying the feeling of control over one's actions ("sense of agency"). Sense of agency may depend on internal motoric signals, and general inferences about external events. We used priming to modulate the sense of agency for voluntary and involuntary movements, by modifying the content of conscious thought prior to moving. Trials began with the presentation of one of two supraliminal primes, which corresponded to the effect of a voluntary action participants subsequently made. The perceived interval between movement and effect was used as an implicit measure of sense of agency. Primes modulated perceived intervals for both voluntary and involuntary movements, but the modulation was greatest for involuntary movements. A second experiment showed that this modulation depended on prime-movement (temporal) contiguity. We propose that sense of agency is based on a combination of internal motoric signals and external sensory evidence about the source of actions and effects.


Assuntos
Estado de Consciência , Sinais (Psicologia) , Ego , Controle Interno-Externo , Julgamento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção do Tempo , Volição , Estimulação Acústica , Conscientização , Feminino , Humanos , Cinestesia , Masculino , Discriminação da Altura Tonal , Propriocepção , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...